Incidental Learning From Science Journalism: The Effects of a Narrative Writing Style on the Comprehension and Situational Interest of Austrian Non-Experts in Health Sciences

Keywords: incidental learning, narrative science journalism, cognitive load, comprehension, situational interest, storytelling

Abstract

Austrian non-experts in health sciences often lack the scientific knowledge to make informed decisions that affect their daily lives and society as a whole. In general, their interest in current scientific knowledge is low, so it is unlikely that they will actively seek out information. Adult non-experts therefore need to acquire relevant scientific information incidentally, e.g., by reading journalistic articles. Journalistic articles traditionally follow an inverted pyramid structure, presenting the most important information first, while narratives unfold events chronologically with intentionally acting characters. Journalistic science narratives provide information that is usually missing in inverted pyramids. This additional information may increase situational interest in journalistic science narratives. Most people are confronted with narratives more often than with explanatory texts in everyday life and can therefore process them better. Due to the linear structure, relevant information elements are automatically activated at the same time, which further facilitates processing. An online study in the context of online science journalism on nutrition topics was conducted in Austria with 461 adult nonexperts in health sciences recruited via online platforms. The dependent variables comprehension, cognitive load and situational interest were investigated using a multiple-tier test and self-reports. The results indicate significantly higher comprehension and general situational interest and significantly lower cognitive load after reading journalistic science narratives compared to inverted pyramids. However, situational interest in journalistic science narratives was significantly lower after participants had read the first paragraph. It then increased and was significantly higher than that for inverted pyramids for the rest of the article. Therefore, journalistic science narratives with a strictly chronological structure should be used with caution. When initial situational interest is low, individuals may stop reading before they have enough information to fully comprehend the content. In addition, higher situational interest is correlated with a lower perception of cognitive load.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

References

Ainley, M., Hidi, S., & Berndorff, D. (2002). Interest, learning, and the psychological processes that mediate their relationship. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(3), 545–561. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.3.545

Ameseder, C., Devetak, I., & Skubic, D. (2020). A study of nutrition knowledge and its relationship with topic interest and preferences for media types among Austrian nonprofessionals in the field of nutrition. In S. Bećirović (Ed.), Conference Proceedings of the Third International Scientific Conference for Doctoral Students and Early Stage Researchers for Educational & Communication Sciences (pp. 23–41). UAS Burgenland. https://purl.org/fhburgenland/9783951993751

Ameseder, C., Skubic, D., & Devetak, I. (2022). Validation of a four-tier test measuring comprehension of journalistic nutrition texts in adult non-experts. In S. Bećirović (Ed.), Conference Proceedings of the Fifth International Scientific Conference for Doctoral Students and Early Stage Researchers for Educational & Communication Sciences (pp. 43–55). UAS Burgenland. https://purl.org/fhburgenland/9783951993799

Angie, A. D., Connelly, S., Waples, E. P., & Kligyte, V. (2011). The influence of discrete emotions on judgement and decision-making: A meta-analytic review. Cognition and Emotion, 25(8), 1393–1422. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.550751

Appel, M., Koch, E., Schreier, M., & Groeben, N. (2002). Aspekte des Leseerlebens: Skalenentwicklung [Aspects of the reading experience: Scale development]. Zeitschrift für Medienpsychologie, 14(4), 149–154.

Arya, D. J., & Maul, A. (2012). The role of the scientific discovery narrative in middle school science education: An experimental study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 1022–1032. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028108

Austrian Academy of Sciences. (2022). Wissenschaftsbarometer Österreich 2022 [Austria Science Barometer 2022]. https://www.oeaw.ac.at/fileadmin/NEWS/2022/PDF/Wissenschaftsbarometer_Oesterreich_c_OeAW.pdf

Avraamidou, L., & Osborne, J. (2009). The role of narrative in communicating science. International Journal of Science Education, 31(12), 1683–1707. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500690802380695

Babyak, M. A., & Green, S. B. (2010). Confirmatory factor analysis: An introduction for psychosomatic medicine researchers. Psychosomatic Medicine, 72(6), 587–597. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181de3f8a

Björnsson, C. H. (1983). Readability of newspapers in 11 languages. Reading Research Quarterly, 18(4), 480–497. https://doi.org/10.2307/747382

Blanca, M. J., Alarcón, R., Arnau, J., Bono, R., & Bendayan, R. (2017). Non-normal data: Is ANOVA still a valid option?. Psicothema, 29(4), 552–557. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.383

Brosius, H. B., Haas, A., & Koschel, F. (2012). Methoden der empirischen Kommunikationsforschung: Eine Einführung [Methods of empirical communication research: An introduction]. Springer VS.

Bortz, J., & Schuster, C. (2010). Statistik für Human- und Sozialwissenschaftler [Statistics for humanand and social scientists] (7th edition). Springer.

Burkart, R. (2002). Kommunikationswissenschaft. Grundlagen und Problemfelder. Umrisse einer interdisziplinären Sozialwissenschaft [Communication science. Fundamentals and problem areas. Outlines of an interdisciplinary social science] (4th edition). UTB.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd edition). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cole, D. A. (1987). Utility of confirmatory factor analysis in test validation research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55(4), 584–594. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.55.4.584

Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 87–185. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01003922

Cunningham, L. J., & Gall, M. D. (1990). The effects of expository and narrative prose on student achievement and attitudes toward textbooks. Journal of Experimental Education, 58(3), 165–175. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.1990.10806532

Dahlstrom, M. F. (2014). Using narratives and storytelling to communicate science with nonexpert audiences. PNAS, 111(4), 13614–13620. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1320645111

DeAngelo, T. I., & Yegiyan, N. S. (2019). Looking for efficiency: How online news structure and emotional tone influence processing time and memory. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 96(2), 385–405. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699018792272

Delgado, P., Vargas, C., Ackerman, R., & Salmerón, L. (2018). Don’t throw away your printed books: A meta-analysis on the effects of reading media on reading comprehension. Educational Research Review, 25, 23–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2018.09.003

Devetak, I., & Glažar, S. A. (2010). The influence of 16-year-old students’ gender, mental abilities, and motivation on their reading and drawing submicrorepresentations achievements. International Journal of Science Education, 32(12), 1561–1593. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500690903150609

Dunwoody, S. (2021). Science journalism. Prospects in the digital age. In M. Bucchi, & B. Trench (Eds.), Routledge handbook of public communication of science and technology (pp. 43–55). Routledge.

Durik, A. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Different strokes for different folks: How individual interest moderates the effects of situational factors on task interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(3), 597–610. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.597

Eaton, P., & Willoughby, S. D. (2018). Confirmatory factor analysis applied to the Force Concept Inventory. Physical Review Physics Education Research, 14(1), Article 010124. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevPhysEducRes.14.010124

Ettl-Huber, S., Ameseder, C., & Pfeiffer, C. (2019). Wie Geschichten wirken. Zum Stand der Wirkungsforschung über Storytelling [How stories work. The state of impact research on storytelling]. In S. Ettl-Huber (Ed.), Storytelling in journalismus, organisations- und marketingkommunikation (pp. 19–43). Springer VS. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-25728-6_2

European Commission. (2010). Special Eurobarometer 340: Science and technology. https://data.europa.eu/euodp/en/data/dataset/S806_73_1_EBS340

European Union. (2021). Special Eurobarometer 516: European citizens’ knowledge and attitudes towards science and technology. https://doi.org/10.2775/071577

Feinstein, N. W., & Waddington, D. I. (2020). Individual truth judgments or purposeful, collective sensemaking? Rethinking science education’s response to the post-truth era. Educational Psychologist, 55(3), 155–166. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2020.1780130

Fernald, L. D. (1989). Tales in a textbook: Learning in the traditional and narrative modes. Teaching of Psychology, 16(3), 121–124. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328023top1603_4

Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2017). Program FACTOR at 10: Origins, development and future directions. Psicothema, 29(2), 236–240. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.304

Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2018). Assessing the quality and appropriateness of factor solutions and factor score estimates in exploratory item factor analysis. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 78(5), 762–780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164417719308

Frey, F. (2014). Wirkungen des Narrativen. Ein systematischer Forschungsüberblick zu Effekten narrativer Kommunikationsformen [A systematic research overview of the effects of narrative forms of communication]. In W. Früh, & F. Frey (Eds.), Narration und Storytelling. Theorie und Empirische Befunde (pp. 120–192). Herbert von Harlem.

Früh, W. (2014) Narration und Storytelling [Narration and storytelling]. In W. Früh, & F. Frey (Eds.), Narration und Storytelling. Theorie und Empirische Befunde (pp. 63–119). Herbert von Harlem.

Früh, W., & Frey, F. (2014). Das Narrative und seine Attraktivitäts- und Wirkungspotenziale. Wirkungen der Definitionskomponenten “Zielgerichtetheit” und “menschlicher Handlungsträger” bei der Filmrezeption [The narrative and its potential for attraction and impact. Effects of the definition components ‘purposefulness’ and ‘human actor’ on film reception]. In W. Früh, & F. Frey (Eds.), Narration und Storytelling. Theorie und Empirische Befunde (pp. 319–371). Herbert von Harlem.

Gensthaler, L., Felsenreich, D. M., Jedamzik, J., Eichetler, J., Nixdorf, L., Bichler, C., Krebs, M., Itariu, B., Langer, F. B., & Prager, G. (2022). Trends of overweight and obesity in male adolescents: Prevalence, socioeconomic status, and impact on cardiovascular risk in a central European country. Obesity Surgery, 32, 1024–1033. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11695-021-05867-z

Grice, J. W. (2001). Computing and evaluating factor scores. Psychological Methods, 6(4), 430–450. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.6.4.430

Gruber, M., Iwuchukwu, C. G., Sperr, E., & König, J. (2022). What do people know about food, nutrition and health? – General nutrition knowledge in the Austrian population. Nutrients, 14(22), Article 4729. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14224729

Gurel, D. K., & Eryılmaz, A. (2015). A review and comparison of diagnostic instruments to identify students’ misconceptions in science. Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education, 11(5), 989–1008. https://doi.org/10.12973/eurasia.2015.1369a

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2014). Multivariate data analysis (7th edition). Pearson Education.

Hendriks, F., Mayweg-Paus, E., Felton, M., Iordanou, K., Jucks, R., & Zimmermann, M. (2020). Constraints and affordances of online engagement with scientific information—A literature review Frontiers in Psychology, 11, Article 572744. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.572744

Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychology, 41(2), 111–27. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep4102_4

Hofstein, A., Eilks, I., & Bybee, R. (2011). Societal issues and their importance for contemporary science education: A pedagogical justification and the state of the art in Israel, Germany and the USA. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 9(6), 1459–1483. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-010-9273-9

Jiang, D., & Kalyuga, S. (2020). Confirmatory factor analysis of cognitive load ratings supports a twofactor model. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 16(3), 216–225. https://doi.org/10.20982/tqmp.16.3.p216

Johnstone, A. H. (1991). Why is science difficult to learn? Things are seldom what they seem. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 7(2), 75–83.

Kaiser, H. F. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika, 39(1), 31–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02291575

Kelly, J., Knight, J., Peck, L. A., & Reel, G. (2003). Straight/narrative? Writing style changes readers’ perceptions of story quality. Newspaper Research Journal, 24(4), 118–122. https://doi.org/10.1177/073953290302400412

Kleemans, M., Schaap, G., & Suijkerbuijk, M. (2018). Getting youngsters hooked on news. Journalism Studies, 19(14), 2108–2125. https://doi.org/10.1080/1461670X.2017.1324316

Klimont, J. (2020). Österreichische Gesundheitsbefragung 2019. Hauptergebnisse des Austrian Health Interview Survey (ATHIS) und methodische Dokumentation [Austrian Health Survey 2019. Main results of the Austrian Health Interview Survey (ATHIS) and methodological documentation]. Statistik Austria. https://www.statistik.at/fileadmin/publications/Oesterreichische-Gesundheitsbefragung2019_Hauptergebnisse.pdf

Knobloch, S., Patzig, G., Mende, A.-M., & Hastall, M. (2004). Affective news. Effects of discourse structure in narratives on suspense, curiosity, and enjoyment while reading news and novels. Communication Research, 31(3), 259–287. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650203261517

Kopp, W. (2019). How western diet and lifestyle drive the pandemic of obesity and civilization diseases. Diabetes, metabolic syndrome and obesity: targets and therapy, 12, 2221–2236. https://doi.org/10.2147%2FDMSO.S216791

Krnel, D., & Skubic, D. (2015). Concept formation in childhood: Cross-cultural perspectives. In J. D. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (2nd edition, vol. 4, pp. 531–537). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23065-1

Lang, A. (2006). Using the limited capacity model of motivated mediated message processing to design effective cancer communication messages. Journal of Communication, 56(Suppl.1), S57– S80. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00283.x

Lawson, A. E., Alkhoury, S., Benford, R., Clark, B. R., & Falconer, K. A. (2000). What kinds of scientific concepts exist? Concept construction and intellectual development in college biology. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(9), 996–1018.

Layng, T. V. J. (2019). Tutorial: Understanding concepts: Implications for behavior analysts and educators. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 42(2), 345–363. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-018-00188-6

Lugmayr, A., Sutinen, E., Suhonen, J., Sedano, C. I., Hlavacs, H., & Montero, C. S. (2017). Serious storytelling – a first definition and review. Multimedia Tools and Applications, 76(14), 15707– 15733. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11042-016-3865-5

Machill, M., Köhler, S., & Waldhauser, M. (2007). The use of narrative structures in television news. European Journal of Communication, 22(2), 185–205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267323107076769

Mar, R. A., Li, J., Nguyen, A. T. P., & Ta, C. P. (2021). Memory and comprehension of narrative versus expository texts: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 28(3), 732–749. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-020-01853-1

Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia learning (2nd edition). Cambridge University Press.

Newman, N., Fletcher, R., Robertson, C. T., Eddy, K., & Nielsen, R. K. (2022). Reuters Institute digital news report 2022. Reuters Institute for the study of Journalism.

Norris, S. P., Guilbert, S. M., Smith, M. L., Hakimelahi, S., & Phillips, L. M. (2005). A theoretical framework for narrative explanation in science. Science Education, 89(4), 535–563. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20063

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2019). PISA 2018 results. https://www.oecd.org/content/dam/oecd/en/about/programmes/edu/pisa/publications/nationalreports/pisa-2018/Combined_Executive_Summaries_PISA_2018.pdf

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2023). PISA 2022 Results (Volume I): The State of Learning and Equity in Education. OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/53f23881-en

Park, B., Moreno, R., Seufert, T., & Brünken, R. (2011). Does cognitive load moderate the seductive details effect? A multimedia study. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 5–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.006

Pudel, V., & Westenhöfer, J. (2003). Ernährungspsychologie. Eine Einführung [Nutritional psychology. An introduction] (3rd edition). Hogrefe.

Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. E. (2019). Interest development and learning. In K. A. Renninger, & S. E. Hidi (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of motivation and learning (pp. 265–290). Cambridge University Press.

Roth, W. M., & Lee, S. (2004). Science education as/for participation in the community. Science Education, 88(2), 263–291. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.10113

Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Cowan, N., Zwilling, C. E., Morey, C. C., & Pratte, M. S. (2008). An assessment of fixed-capacity models of visual working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(16), 5975–5979. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0711295105

Rust, P., Hasenegger, V., & König, J. (2017). Österreichischer Ernährungsbericht 2017 [Austrian Nutrition Report 2017]. University of Vienna.

Schiefele, U. (1996). Motivation und Lernen mit Texten [Motivation and learning with texts]. Hogrefe.

Schiefele, U., & Krapp, A. (1996). Topic interest and free recall of expository text. Learning and Individual Differences, 8(2), 141–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1041-6080(96)90030-8

Schmeck, A., Opfermann, M., van Gog, T., Paas, F., & Leutner, D. (2015). Measuring cognitive load with subjective rating scales during problem solving: differences between immediate and delayed ratings. Instructional Science, 43(1), 93–114. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-014-9328-3

Schnotz, W., & Kürschner, C. (2007). A reconsideration of cognitive load theory. Educational Psychology Review, 19(4), 469–508. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-007-9053-4

Shen, F., Ahern, L., & Baker, M. (2014). Stories that count: Influence of news narratives on issue attitudes. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 91(1), 98–117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699013514414

Shulman, H. C., & Sweitzer, M. D. (2017). Varying metacognition through public opinion questions: How language can affect political engagement. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 37(2), 224–237. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X17707557

Slapničar, M., Ribič, L., Devetak, I., & Vinko, L. (2024). Inquiry-based chemistry education activities in a non-formal educational setting for gifted students. Center for Educational Policy Studies Journal, 14(1), 143–169. https://doi.org/10.26529/cepsj.1706

Slater, M. D. (1997). Persuasion processes across receiver goals and message genres. Communication Theory, 7(2), 125–148. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.1997.tb00145.x

Snětinová, M., Kacovsky, P., & Machalicka, J. (2018). Hands-on experiments in the interactive physics laboratory: Students’ intrinsic motivation and understanding. Center for Educational Policy Studies Journal, 8(1), 55–75. https://doi.org/10.26529/cepsj.319

Sørensen, K., Pelikan, J. M., Röthlin, F., Ganahl, K., Slonska, Z., Doyle, G., Fullam, J., Kondilis, B., Agrafiotis, D., Uiters, E., Falcon, M., Mensing, M., Tchamov, K., van den Broucke, S., & Brand, H. (2015). Health literacy in Europe: Comparative results of the European health literacy survey (HLSEU). European Journal of Public Health, 25(6), 1053–1058. https://doi.org/10.1093/eurpub/ckv043

Statistik Austria. (2024). IKT-Einsatz in Haushalten 2023 [ICT use in households 2023]. Retrieved September 16, 2024, from https://www.statistik.at/fileadmin/user_upload/IKTH_2023.pdf

Sternadori, M. (2008). Cognitive processing of news as a function of structure: A comparison between inverted pyramid and chronology. [Doctoral dissertation, University of Missouri-Columbia]. University of Missouri-Columbia. https://mospace.umsystem.edu/xmlui/handle/10355/6643

Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-8126-4

Thorndyke, P. W. (1977). Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive Psychology, 9(1), 77–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90005-6

van den Broek, P. W. (2010). Using texts in science education: Cognitive processes and knowledge representation. Science, 328(5977), 453–456. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1182594

van den Broek, P. W., Risden, K., Fletcher, C. R., & Thurlow, R. (1996). A “landscape” view of reading: Fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a stable memory representation. In B. K. Britton, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp. 165–187). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

van Dijk, T. A. (1986). News as discourse. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

van Krieken, K., Hoeken, H., & Sanders, J. (2015). From reader to mediated witness: The engaging effects of journalistic crime narratives. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 92(3), 580–596. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077699015586546

Wagnon, C. C., Wehrmann, K., Klöppel, S., & Peter, J. (2019). Incidental learning: A systematic review of its effect on episodic memory performance in older age. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 11, 173. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2019.00173

Weitze, M.-D., & Heckl, W. M. (2016). Wissenschaftskommunikation. Schlüsselbeispiele, Akteure, Fallbeispiele [Science communication. Key examples, actors, case studies]. Springer.

Wise, K., Bolls, P., Myers, J., & Sternadori, M. (2009). When words collide online: How writing style and video intensity affect cognitive processing of online news. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 53(4), 532–546. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838150903333023

Yaros, R. A. (2006). Is it the medium or the message? Structuring complex news to enhance engagement and situational understanding by nonexperts. Communication Research, 33(4), 285–309. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650206289154

Yaros, R. A. (2011). Effects of text and hypertext structures on user interest and understanding of science and technology. Science Communication, 33(3), 275–308. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547010386803

Yaros, R. A., & Cook, A. E. (2013). Attention versus learning of online content. In R. Zheng (Ed.), Evolving psychological and educational perspectives on cyber behavior (pp. 132–154). Information Science Reference. https://doi.org/10.4018/978-1-4666-1858-9.ch009

Zabrucky, K. M., & Moore, D. (1999). Influence of text genre on adults’ monitoring of understanding and recall. Educational Gerontology, 25(8), 691–710. https://doi.org/10.1080/036012799267440

Zerba, A. (2008). Narrative storytelling: Putting the story back in hard news to engage young audiences. Newspaper Research Journal, 29(3), 94–102.

Published
2025-01-22
How to Cite
Ameseder, C., Devetak, I., & Skubic, D. (2025). Incidental Learning From Science Journalism: The Effects of a Narrative Writing Style on the Comprehension and Situational Interest of Austrian Non-Experts in Health Sciences. Center for Educational Policy Studies Journal. https://doi.org/10.26529/cepsj.1899